This Idea Must Die: Some of the World's Greatest Thinkers Each Select a Major Misconception Holding Us Back
"To kill an error is as good a service as, and sometimes even better than, the establishing of a new truth or fact,"asserted Charles Darwin in one of the eleven rules for critical thinking known asProspero's Precepts. If science and human knowledge progress in leaps and bounds of ignorance, then the recognition of error and the transcendence of falsehood are the springboard for the leaps of progress. That's the premise behind This Idea Must Die: Scientific Theories That Are Blocking Progress(public library) – a compendium of answers Edge founder John Brockmancollected by posing his annual question –"What scientific idea is ready for retirement?" – to 175 of the world's greatest scientists, philosophers, and writers. Among them are Nobel laureates, MacArthur geniuses, and celebrated minds like theoretical physicist and mathematicianFreeman Dyson, biological anthropologist Helen Fisher, cognitive scientist and linguist Steven Pinker, media theorist Douglas Rushkoff, philosopher Rebecca Newberger Goldstein, psychologistHoward Gardner, social scientist and technology scholar Sherry Turkle, actor and author Alan Alda, futurist and Wired founding editorKevin Kelly, and novelist, essayist, and screenwriter Ian McEwan.
Brockman paints the backdrop for the inquiry:
Science advances by discovering new things and developing new ideas. Few truly new ideas are developed without abandoning old ones first. As theoretical physicist Max Planck (1858–1947) noted, “A new scientific truth does not triumph by convincing its opponents and making them see the light, but rather because its opponents eventually die, and a new generation grows up that is familiar with it.” In other words, science advances by a series of funerals.
Many of the answers are redundant – but this is a glorious feature rather than a bug of Brockman's series, for its chief reward is precisely this cumulative effect of discerning the zeitgeist of ideas with which some of our era's greatest minds are tussling in synchronicity. They point to such retirement-ready ideas as IQ, the self, race, the left brain vs. right brain divide, human nature and essentialism, free will, and even science itself. What emerges is the very thing Carl Sagan deemed vital to truth in his Baloney Detection Kit – a "substantive debate on the evidence by knowledgeable proponents of all points of view."
Illustration by Lizi Boyd from Flashlight
One of the most profound undercurrents across the answers has to do with our relationship with knowledge, certainty, and science itself. And one of the most profound contributions in that regard comes from MacArthur fellow Rebecca Newberger Goldstein, a philosopher who thinks deeply and dimensionally about some of the most complex questions of existence. Assailing the idea that science makes philosophy obsolete – that science is the transformation of "philosophy’s vagaries into empirically testable theories" and philosophy merely the "cold-storage room in which questions are shelved until the sciences get around to handling them" – Goldstein writes:
The obsolescence of philosophy is often taken to be a consequence of science. After all, science has a history of repeatedly inheriting – and definitively answering – questions over which philosophers have futilely hemmed and hawed for unconscionable amounts of time.
The gravest problem with this theory, Goldstein notes, is its internal incoherence:
You can’t argue for science making philosophy obsolete without indulging in philosophical arguments... When pressed for an answer to the so-called demarcation problem, scientists almost automatically reach for the notion of “falsifiability” first proposed by Karl Popper. His profession? Philosophy. But whatever criterion you offer, its defense is going to implicate you in philosophy.
This is something that Dorion Sagan, Carl Sagan's son, has previously addressed, but Goldstein brings to it unparalleled elegance of thought and eloquence of expression:
A triumphalist scientism needs philosophy to support itself. And the lesson here should be generalized. Philosophy is joined to science in reason’s project. Its mandate is to render our views and our attitudes maximally coherent.
In doing so, she argues, philosophy provides "the reasoning that science requires in order to claim its image as descriptive." As a proponent of the vital difference between information and wisdom – the former being the material of science, the latter the product of philosophy, and knowledge the change agent that transmutes one into the other – I find the provocative genius of Goldstein's conclusion enormously invigorating:
What idea should science retire? The idea of “science” itself. Let’s retire it in favor of the more inclusive “knowledge.”
In a further testament to the zeitgeist-illuminating nature of the project, actor, author, and science-lover Alan Alda makes a passionate case for the same concept:
The trouble with truth is that not only is the notion of eternal, universal truth highly questionable, but simple, local truths are subject to refinement as well. Up is up and down is down, of course. Except under special circumstances. Is the North Pole up and the South Pole down? Is someone standing at one of the poles right-side up or upside-down? Kind of depends on your perspective.When I studied how to think in school, I was taught that the first rule of logic was that a thing cannot both be and not be at the same time and in the same respect. That last note, “in the same respect,” says a lot. As soon as you change the frame of reference, you’ve changed the truthiness of a once immutable fact.[...]This is not to say that nothing is true or that everything is possible – just that it might not be so helpful for things to be known as true for all time, without a disclaimer... I wonder – and this is just a modest proposal – whether scientific truth should be identified in a way acknowledging that it’s something we know and understand for now, and in a certain way.[...]Facts, it seems to me, are workable units, useful in a given frame or context. They should be as exact and irrefutable as possible, tested by experiment to the fullest extent. When the frame changes, they don’t need to be discarded as untrue but respected as still useful within their domain. Most people who work with facts accept this, but I don’t think the public fully gets it.That’s why I hope for more wariness about implying we know something to be true or false for all time and for everywhere in the cosmos.
Illustration from Once Upon an Alphabet by Oliver Jeffers
And indeed this elasticity of truth across time is at the heart of what I find to be the most beautiful and culturally essential contribution to the collection. As someone who believes that the stewardship of enduring ideas is at least as important as the genesis of new ones – not only because past ideas are the combinatorial building blocks of future ones but also because in order to move forward we always need a backdrop against which to paint the contrast of progress and improvement – I was most bewitched by writer Ian McEwan’s admonition against the arrogance of retiring any idea as an impediment to progress:
Beware of arrogance! Retire nothing! A great and rich scientific tradition should hang onto everything it has. Truth is not the only measure. There are ways of being wrong that help others to be right. Some are wrong, but brilliantly so. Some are wrong but contribute to method. Some are wrong but help found a discipline. Aristotle ranged over the whole of human knowledge and was wrong about much. But his invention of zoology alone was priceless. Would you cast him aside? You never know when you might need an old idea. It could rise again one day to enhance a perspective the present cannot imagine. It would not be available to us if it were fully retired.
To appreciate McEwan's point, one need only look at something likeBertrand Russell's timely thoughts on boredom, penned in 1930 and yet astoundingly resonant with our present anxieties about the societal side effects of current technology. McEwan captures this beautifully:
Every last serious and systematic speculation about the world deserves to be preserved. We need to remember how we got to where we are, and we’d like the future not to retire us. Science should look to literature and maintain a vibrant living history as a monument to ingenuity and persistence. We won’t retire Shakespeare. Nor should we Bacon.
Illustration by Rob Hunter from A Graphic Cosmogony
Neuroscientist Sam Harris, author of the indispensable Spirituality Without Religion, echoes this by choosing our narrow definition of "science" as the idea to be put to rest:
Search your mind, or pay attention to the conversations you have with other people, and you’ll discover that there are no real boundaries between science and philosophy – or between those disciplines and any other that attempts to make valid claims about the world on the basis of evidence and logic. When such claims and their methods of verification admit of experiment and/or mathematical description, we tend to say our concerns are “scientific”; when they relate to matters more abstract, or to the consistency of our thinking itself, we often say we’re being “philosophical”; when we merely want to know how people behaved in the past, we dub our interests “historical” or “journalistic”; and when a person’s commitment to evidence and logic grows dangerously thin or simply snaps under the burden of fear, wishful thinking, tribalism, or ecstasy, we recognize that he’s being “religious.”The boundaries between true intellectual disciplines are currently enforced by little more than university budgets and architecture... The real distinction we should care about – the observation of which is thesine qua non of the scientific attitude – is between demanding good reasons for what one believes and being satisfied with bad ones.
In a sentiment that calls to mind both Richard Feynman's spectacularode to a flower and Carl Sagan's enduring wisdom on our search for meaning, Harris applies this model of knowledge to one of the great mysteries of science and philosophy – consciousness:
Even if one thinks the human mind is entirely the product of physics, the reality of consciousness becomes no less wondrous, and the difference between happiness and suffering no less important. Nor does such a view suggest that we’ll ever find the emergence of mind from matter fully intelligible; consciousness may always seem like a miracle. In philosophical circles, this is known as “the hard problem of consciousness” – some of us agree that this problem exists, some of us don’t. Should consciousness prove conceptually irreducible, remaining the mysterious ground for all we can conceivably experience or value, the rest of the scientific worldview would remain perfectly intact.The remedy for all this confusion is simple: We must abandon the idea that science is distinct from the rest of human rationality. When you are adhering to the highest standards of logic and evidence, you are thinking scientifically. And when you’re not, you’re not.
Illustration from Once Upon an Alphabet by Oliver Jeffers
In a related grievance, social psychologist Bruce Hood – author of the uncomfortable yet strangely comforting The Self Illusion – does away with the notion of the self. Half a century after Alan Watts enlisted Eastern philosophy in this mission, Hood presents a necessary integration of science and philosophy:
It seems almost redundant to call for the retirement of the free willing self, as the idea is neither scientific nor is this the first time the concept has been dismissed for lack of empirical support. The self did not have to be discovered; it’s the default assumption most of us experience, so it wasn’t really revealed by methods of scientific inquiry.[...]Yet the self, like a conceptual zombie, refuses to die. It crops up again and again in recent theories of decision making, as an entity with free will which can be depleted. It reappears as an interpreter in cognitive neuroscience, as able to integrate parallel streams of information arising from separable neural substrates. Even if these appearances of the self are understood to be convenient ways of discussing the emergent output of multiple parallel processes, students of the mind continue to implicitly endorse the idea that there’s a decision maker, an experiencer, a point of origin.We know the self is constructed because it can be so easily deconstructed – through damage, disease, and drugs. It must be an emergent property of a parallel system processing input, output, and internal representations. It’s an illusion because it feels so real, but that experience is not what it seems. The same is true for free will. Although we can experience the mental anguish of making a decision... the choices and decisions we make are based on situations that impose on us. We don’t have the free will to choose the experiences that have shaped our decisions.[...]By abandoning the free willing self, we’re forced to reexamine the factors that are truly behind our thoughts and behavior and the way they interact, balance, override, and cancel out. Only then will we begin to make progress in understanding how we really operate.
Illustration by Ben Newman from A Graphic Cosmogony
Among the most provocative answers, in fact, is one examining the factors that underlie one of the most complex and seemingly human of our experiences: love. Biological anthropologist Helen Fisher, who studies the brain on love, points to romantic love and addiction as two concepts in need of serious reformulation and reframing – one best accomplished by understanding the intersection of the two. Fisher argues that we ought to broaden the definition of addiction and do away with science's staunch notion that all addiction is harmful. Love, she argues with a wealth of neurobiological evidence in hand, is in fact a state that closely resembles that of addiction in terms of what happens in the brain during it – and yet love, anguishing as it may be at times, is universally recognized as the height of positive experience. In that respect, it presents a case of "positive addiction." Fisher writes:
Love-besotted men and women show all the basic symptoms of addiction. Foremost, the lover is stiletto-focused on his/her drug of choice, the love object. The lover thinks obsessively about him or her (intrusive thinking), and often compulsively calls, writes, or stays in touch. Paramount in this experience is intense motivation to win one’s sweetheart, not unlike the substance abuser fixated on the drug. Impassioned lovers distort reality, change their priorities and daily habits to accommodate the beloved, experience personality changes (affect disturbance), and sometimes do inappropriate or risky things to impress this special other. Many are willing to sacrifice, even die for, “him” or “her.” The lover craves emotional and physical union with the beloved (dependence). And like addicts who suffer when they can’t get their drug, the lover suffers when apart from the beloved (separation anxiety). Adversity and social barriers even heighten this longing (frustration attraction).In fact, besotted lovers express all four of the basic traits of addiction: craving, tolerance, withdrawal, and relapse. They feel a “rush” of exhilaration when they’re with their beloved (intoxication). As their tolerance builds, they seek to interact with the beloved more and more (intensification). If the love object breaks off the relationship, the lover experiences signs of drug withdrawal, including protest, crying spells, lethargy, anxiety, insomnia or hypersomnia, loss of appetite or binge eating, irritability, and loneliness. Lovers, like addicts, also often go to extremes, sometimes doing degrading or physically dangerous things to win back the beloved. And lovers relapse the way drug addicts do. Long after the relationship is over, events, people, places, songs, or other external cues associated with their abandoning sweetheart can trigger memories and renewed craving.
Fisher points to fMRI studies that have shown intense romantic love to trigger the brain's reward system and the dopamine pathways responsible for "energy, focus, motivation, ecstasy, despair, and craving," as well as the brain regions most closely associated with addiction and substance abuse. In shedding light on the neurochemical machinery of romantic love, Fisher argues, science reveals it to be a "profoundly powerful, natural, often positive addiction."
Illustration by Christine Rösch from The Mathematics of Love by Hannah Fry
Science writer Amanda Gefter takes issue with one particular manifestation of our propensity for oversimplification – the notion of theuniverse. She writes:
Physics has a time-honored tradition of laughing in the face of our most basic intuitions. Einstein’s relativity forced us to retire our notions of absolute space and time, while quantum mechanics forced us to retire our notions of pretty much everything else. Still, one stubborn idea has stood steadfast through it all: the universe.[...]In recent years, however, the concept of a single shared spacetime has sent physics spiraling into paradox. The first sign that something was amiss came from Stephen Hawking’s landmark work in the 1970s showing that black holes radiate and evaporate, disappearing from the universe and purportedly taking some quantum information with them. Quantum mechanics, however, is predicated upon the principle that information can never be lost.
Gefter points to recent breakthroughs in physics that produced one particularly puzzling such paradox, known as the "firewall paradox," solved by the idea that spacetime is divided not by horizons but by the reference frames of the observers, "as if each observer had his or her own universe."
But the solution isn't a multiverse theory:
Yes, there are multiple observers, and yes, any observer’s universe is as good as any other’s. But if you want to stay on the right side of the laws of physics, you can talk only about one at a time. Which means, really, that only one exists at a time. It’s cosmic solipsism.
Ceramic tile by Debbie Millman courtesy of the artist
Here, psychology, philosophy, and cosmology converge, for what such theories suggest is what we already know about the human psyche – as I've put it elsewhere, the stories that we tell ourselves, whether they be false or true, are always real. Gefter concludes:
Adjusting our intuitions and adapting to the strange truths uncovered by physics is never easy. But we may just have to come around to the notion that there’s my universe and there’s your universe – but there’s no such thing as the universe.
Psychologist Jonathan Gottschall, who has previously explored why storytelling is so central to the human experience, argues against the notion that there can be no science of art. With an eye to our civilization's long struggle to define art, he writes:
We don’t even have a good definition, in truth, for what art is. In short, there’s nothing so central to human life that’s so incompletely understood.
Granted, Gottschall is only partly right, for there are some excellent definitions of art – take, for instance, Jeanette Winterson's or Leo Tolstoy's – but the fact that they don't come from scientists only speaks to his larger point. He argues that rather than being unfit to shed light on the role of art in human life, science simply hasn't applied itself to the problem adequately:
Scientific work in the humanities has mainly been scattered, preliminary, and desultory. It doesn’t constitute a research program.If we want better answers to fundamental questions about art, science must jump into the game with both feet. Going it alone, humanities scholars can tell intriguing stories about the origins and significance of art, but they don’t have the tools to patiently winnow the field of competing ideas. That’s what the scientific method is for – separating the more accurate stories from the less accurate stories. But a strong science of art will require both the thick, granular expertise of humanities scholars and the clever hypothesis-testing of scientists. I’m not calling for a scientific takeover of the arts, I’m calling for a partnership.[...]The Delphic admonition “Know thyself” still rings out as the great prime directive of intellectual inquiry, and there will always be a gaping hole in human self-knowledge until we...
No comments:
Post a Comment