Pages

Saturday, July 12, 2014

Frontline: A Rape-Murder Case Involving A Daisy Chain Of 4 False Confessions

Alan: "The Confessions" is an ideal companion piece to "Death by Fire," a Frontline episode probing the false conviction and subsequent execution of Cameron Todd Willingham. http://paxonbothhouses.blogspot.com/2014/07/conservative-political-philosophy-led.html Scientific experiment shows that 60% of study participants can be persuaded to give false confessions. http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/False_confession Innocent people are also convicted because "false judgment" and "rush to judgment" are deemed better than "no judgment," particularly in cases where egregious crime has been committed. I am reminded of the phrase, "If the devil did not exist, we would invent him." In the absence of evidence demonstrating "guilt beyond reasonable doubt," we humans create "evidence." We simply "feel better" when our beliefs are "evident" and, as a nation, enshrine our passion for "self-justification" in the lofty (but dubious) phrase "We hold these truths to be self-evident." 

***

"Scalia Pushed Death Penalty For Now-Exonerated Inmate Henry Lee McCollum"

http://paxonbothhouses.blogspot.com/2014/09/scalia-pushed-death-penalty-for-now.html

"Duped by Medill Innocence Project, Milwaukee Man Now Free"

http://paxonbothhouses.blogspot.com/2014/11/duped-by-medill-innocence-project.html

"More DNA Exonerations: 20% Of People Who Confess ToCapital Crimes Are Coerced"


Here is my personal account of the systematic attempt by law enforcement to create "legal" evidence where there is none:  
"After My Mugging c. 1970, The Police Pressured Me To Give False Testimony"
http://paxonbothhouses.blogspot.com/2014/07/after-my-mugging-in-1970-police.html

***

The Confessions

How could four men confess to a brutal crime that they didn't commit? Inside the incredible saga of the Norfolk Four -- a case that cracks open the justice system to reveal almost everything that goes wrong when innocent people get convicted.


From Wikipedia's "False Confession" entry:
False confessions can be categorized into three general types, as outlined by Saul M. Kassin in an article for Current Directions in Psychological Science:[2]
  • Voluntary false confessions are those that are given freely, without police prompting. Sometimes they may be sacrificial, to divert attention from the actual person who committed the crime. For instance, a parent might confess to save their child from jail. In some cases, people have falsely confessed to having committed notorious crimes simply for the attention that they receive from such a confession. Approximately 60 people are reported to have confessed to the 1947 murder of Elizabeth Short, known as the "Black Dahlia."[3]
  • Compliant false confessions are given to escape a stressful situation, avoid punishment, or gain a promised or implied reward. Take, for example, the setting of a police interrogation, which is simply total isolation. Interrogations are often conducted in stark rooms with no windows and no objects other than perhaps a table and two chairs. For suspects, the room becomes reality, and this creates serious mental exhaustion for the individual being questioned. After enough time suspects may confess to crimes they did not commit to escape what feels like a helpless situation. Interrogation techniques such as the Reid technique try to suggest to the suspect that he will experience a feeling of moral appeasement if he chooses to confess. Material rewards like coffee or the cessation of the interrogation are also used to the same effect. People may also confess to a crime they did not commit as a form of plea bargaining to avoid a harsher sentence. People who are easily coerced score high on the Gudjonsson suggestibility scale.
  • Internalized false confessions are those in which the person genuinely believes that they have committed the crime, as a result of highly suggestive interrogation techniques.
According to the Innocence Project, approximately 25% of convicted criminals ultimately exonerated had, in fact, confessed to the crime.[4] In Canada, courts of law have recognized as valid confessions that were acquired, even though the interrogators lied by suggesting they had substantial evidence against a given suspect when in fact they did not, something known as the "bluff" technique.[5] The high pressure generated may push innocent individuals to produce a confession.[6]
A 2010 study from CUNY's John Jay College of Criminal Justice used laboratory experiments that test how the bluff technique correlates with confessions gained from innocent parties. Subjects were instructed to complete a task on a computer, then were falsely accused of a transgression such as crashing the computer or collaborating with a colleague to improve their task performance.[7] Bluff evidence, false evidence, and unreliable witnesses were used to test their effect. In the first test, 60% of the subjects confessed to the experimenter to pressing a computer key they had been instructed to avoid when, in fact, they had not; an additional 10% admitted to pressing the key to a study observer. A second group that tested subject reactions to charges of cheating produced nearly identical percentages of false confessions. The authors note, "innocent people who stand accused believe that their innocence will become apparent to others ... which leads them to waive their Miranda right to silence and to an attorney."[7]
False confessions greatly undermine the due process rights of the individual who has confessed. As Justice Brennan noted in his dissent in Colorado v. Connelly,[8] "Our distrust for reliance on confessions is due, in part, to their decisive impact upon the adversarial process. Triers of fact accord confessions such heavy weight in their determinations that 'the introduction of a confession makes the other aspects of a trial in court superfluous, and the real trial, for all practical purposes, occurs when the confession is obtained.' No other class of evidence is so profoundly prejudicial. 'Thus the decision to confess before trial amounts in effect to a waiver of the right to require the state at trial to meet its heavy burden of proof.'"
Coerced false confessions have been used for directly political purposes. The systematic use of coerced confessions of political prisoners to extract public recantations for propaganda purposes has occurred in the twentieth (and twenty first) century in Stalin's Soviet Union, Maoist China, and most recently the Islamic Republic of Iran.[9]

***

Here is the  postscript to an email I sent to two friends in April, 2012.

This morning NPR broadcast a stunning report about miscarriage of justice - http://www.npr.org/2012/04/28/150996459/free-after-25-years-a-tale-of-murder-and-injustice 

As a courtroom interpreter -- and a victim of violent crime once pushed by Chief Detective Fantagrossi (of the Rochester, New York Police Department) to identify someone who looked vaguely like one of my assailants - I am appallingly aware that district attorneys and police departments are "out to get somebody." Often their prosecutorial target does not matter, so long as they "get somebody." I understand the urge for "punishment" and "closure" but I also understand how quickly urges become habitual regardless their larger ramifications. Consider this similar situation... Were it not for friend Steve Gibson's family wealth (and consequent ability to re-enact a crime in court, complete with a screen door and duplicated "light conditions"), Steve would have languished in an Illinois prison for armed robbery. 

Michael Morton and his mother, Patricia Morton, in October after a judge announced him free on bond after nearly 25 years in prison for a wrongful conviction.

Free After 25 Years: A Tale Of Murder And Injustice

Michael Morton was convicted of killing his wife and put in prison for life. DNA evidence finally freed him, but it took a quarter-century to force Texas officials to reveal the evidence that exonerated him.
***

Alan: Here is another version of my encounter with Chief Detective Fantagrossi:

When I was a young man driving cab in Rochester, New York, I was mugged by two assailants brandishing gun, knife and an unknown “blinding” caustic.

Several hours after my release from Genesee Hospital I was “called downtown” to view a police lineup.

I confided to Chief Detective Fantagrossi that I would never be able to swear to the identity of any of the suspects, also mentioning that the guy who stood second from the left bore an approximate resemblance.

At that point, Fantagrossi leaned into me physically, and like a nuzzling mammal, cooed,: “Oh, I’m sure you can.”

From the vantage of law enforcement (and much of our legal apparatus) the “usual suspects” are “guilty of something” so let’s use any available pretext to “get ‘em off the streets.”


No comments:

Post a Comment